Imagery vividness and perspectives in women’s artistic gymnastics training
Main Article Content
Abstract
This study aimed to examine imagery vividness and different modalities and perspectives in women’s artistic gymnastics. It explored athletes’ imagery use in training practice and how vivid imagery is developed for different motor tasks. Forty-seven female athletes (age 19 ± 3 years), with women’s artistic gymnastics expertise (12 ± 4 years) participated in this study. The Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire (Dahm et al., 2019) was used to determine the imagery vividness of the participants for the different imagery modalities and perspectives respectively: external–visual, internal–visual, and kinaesthetic. The ANOVA results showed no differences between the most vivid imagery for age (F[3] = 0.724, p = .54) and expertise (F[4] = 0.091, p = .97). T-test results revealed differences in the imagery vividness of the kinaesthetic imagery depending on the gymnasts’ activity as a coach (F[45] = 5.280, p < .05). To benefit from imagery use in training (e.g. to adjust coaching), the most vivid imagery modality and perspective must be determined individually. These results emphasise the need for skill-specific assessments of imagery modalities and perspectives to benefit from individualised imagery-adapted instructions.
Downloads
Article Details
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
Each author warrants that his or her submission to the Work is original and that he or she has full power to enter into this agreement. Neither this Work nor a similar work has been published elsewhere in any language nor shall be submitted for publication elsewhere while under consideration by Journal of Human Sport and Exercise (JHSE). Each author also accepts that the JHSE will not be held legally responsible for any claims of compensation.
Authors wishing to include figures or text passages that have already been published elsewhere are required to obtain permission from the copyright holder(s) and to include evidence that such permission has been granted when submitting their papers. Any material received without such evidence will be assumed to originate from the authors.
Please include at the end of the acknowledgements a declaration that the experiments comply with the current laws of the country in which they were performed. The editors reserve the right to reject manuscripts that do not comply with the abovementioned requirements. The author(s) will be held responsible for false statements or failure to fulfill the above-mentioned requirements.
This title is licensed under a Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0).
You are free to:
Share — copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format.
Adapt — remix, transform, and build upon the material.
The licensor cannot revoke these freedoms as long as you follow the license terms.
Under the following terms:
-
Attribution — You must give appropriate credit, provide a link to the license, and indicate if changes were made. You may do so in any reasonable manner, but not in any way that suggests the licensor endorses you or your use.
-
NonCommercial — You may not use the material for commercial purposes.
-
ShareAlike — If you remix, transform, or build upon the material, you must distribute your contributions under the same license as the original.
- No additional restrictions — You may not apply legal terms or technological measures that legally restrict others from doing anything the license permits.
Notices:
- You do not have to comply with the license for elements of the material in the public domain or where your use is permitted by an applicable exception or limitation.
- No warranties are given. The license may not give you all of the permissions necessary for your intended use. For example, other rights such as publicity, privacy, or moral rights may limit how you use the material.
How to Cite
References
Arvinen-Barrow, M., Weigand, D., Thomas, S., Hemmings, B., & Walley, M. (2007). Elite and Novice Athletes' Imagery Use in Open and Closed Sports. J Appl Sport Psychol, 19, 93-104. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200601102912
Berisha, M., & Mosier, W. (2020). The psychology of motor learning in artistic gymnastics. In Özen, G. & Dindar, M. (Eds.), Multidisciplinary Research in Sports Sciences II (Chapter 31, pp. 505-529). Istanbul: Efe Akademi Publisher.
Callow, N., Roberts, R., Hardy, L., Jiang, D., & Edwards, M. (2013). Performance improvements from imagery: Evidence that internal visual imagery is superior to external visual imagery for slalom performance. Front Hum Neurosci. 7. 697. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00697
Coelho, R.W., De Campos, W., Da Silva, S.G., Okazaki, F.H., Keller, B. (2007). Imagery intervention in open and closed tennis motor skill performance. Percept Mot Skills. Oct; 105(2): 458-68. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.105.2.458-468
Cumming, J., & Williams, S. E. (2013). Introducing the revised applied model of deliberate imagery use for sport, dance, exercise, and rehabilitation. Mov Sport Sci/Sci Mot(82), 69-81. https://doi.org/10.1051/sm/2013098
Dahm, S.F., Bart, V.K.E., Pithan, J.M., & Rieger, M. (2019). Deutsche Übersetzung und Validierung des VMIQ-2 zur Erfassung der Lebhaftigkeit von Handlungsvorstellungen. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 26(4): 151-158. https://doi.org/10.1026/1612-5010/a000273
Dhouibi, M-A., Miladi, I., Racil, G., Hammoudi, S., & Coquart, J. (2021). The Effects of Sporting and Physical Practice on Visual and Kinesthetic Motor Imagery Vividness: A Comparative Study Between Athletic, Physically Active, and Exempted Adolescents. Front Psychol. 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.776833
Guillot, A., Collet, C., & Dittmar, A. (2004). Relationship between visual and kinesthetic imagery, field dependence-independence, and complex motor skills. J Psychophys, 18, 190-198. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.18.4.190
Hardy, L., & Callow, N. (1999) Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery perspectives for the enhancement of performance on tasks in which form is important. J Sport Exerc Psychol, 21, 95-112. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.21.2.95
Ladda, A.M., Lebon, F., & Lotze, M. (2021). Using motor imagery practice for improving motor performance - A review. Brain Cogn, 150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105705
Liu, K.P.Y., Lai, M., Fong, S., & Bissett, M. (2019). Imagery Ability and Imagery Perspective Preference: A Study of Their Relationship and Age- and Gender-Related Changes. Behav Neurol, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1155/2019/7536957
Mahoney, M.J., & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the elite athlete: an exploratory study. Cogn Ther Res, 3, 361-366. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173634
Moran, A., Campbell, M., Holmes, P., & Macintyre, T. (2012). Mental Imagery, Action Observation and Skill Learning. In N. J. Hodges, & A. M. Williams (Eds.), Skill Acquisition in Sport- Research, Theory and Practice (2nd ed., pp. 94-111). Routledge (Taylor and Francis).
Munzert, J., & Lorey, B. (2013). Motor and Visual Imagery in Sports. In: S. Lacey, & R. Lawson (Eds.), Multisensory Imagery (pp. 319-341). Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5879-1_17
Prastyawan, R. R., Suherman, W. S., & Nasrulloh, A. (2023). The influence of internal and external visual imagery on self-confidence in learning gymnastics. Cakrawala Pendidikan: Journal Ilmiah Pendidikan, 42(3), 771-780. https://doi.org/10.21831/cp.v42i3.56903
Roberts, R., Callow, N., Hardy, L., Markland, D., & Bringer, J. (2008). Movement Imagery Ability: Development and Assessment of a Revised Version of the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire. J Sport Exerc Psychol, 30, 200-221. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.30.2.200
Schmidt RA and Lee T. Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 2005.
Simonsmeier, B.A., Androniea, M., Buecker, S., & Frank, C. (2020). The effects of imagery interventions in sports: a meta-analysis. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2020.1780627
Simonsmeier, B.A., & Frank, C. (2016). Imagery use ability and performance. In: T. Heinen, I. Cuk, R. Goebel, & K. Velentzas (Eds.), Gymnastics performance and motor learning (pp. 53-69). New York: NOVA.
Subirats, L., Allali, G., Briansoulet, M., Salle, J.Y., & Perrochon, A. (2018). Age and gender differences in motor imagery. J Neurol Sci, 391, 114-117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2018.06.015
Ungerleider, S., & Golding, J.M. (1991). Mental practice among Olympic athletes. Percept Mot Skills, 72, 1007-1017. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1991.72.3.1007
White, A., & Hardy, L. (1995). Use of different imagery perspectives on the learning and performance of different motor skills. Br J Psychol, 86(2), 169-180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02554.x
Wright, D. J., Frank, C. & Bruton, A. M. (2022). Recommendations for Combining Action Observation and Motor Imagery Interventions in Sport, J Sport Psychol Action, 13(3), 155-167. https://doi.org/10.1080/21520704.2021.1971810